Article  
i-Perception  
May-June 2017, 1–16  
The ‘‘Spinner’’ Illusion:  
More Dots, More Speed?  
Hiroshi Ashida  
Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan  
Alan Ho  
Ambrose University, Alberta, Canada  
Akiyoshi Kitaoka  
Ritsumeikan University, Kyoto, Japan  
Stuart Anstis  
University of California, San Diego, CA, USA  
Abstract  
The perceived speed of a ring of equally spaced dots moving around a circular path appears faster  
as the number of dots increases (Ho & Anstis, 2013, Best Illusion of the Year contest). We  
measured this ‘‘spinner’’ effect with radial sinusoidal gratings, using a 2AFC procedure where  
participants selected the faster one between two briefly presented gratings of different spatial  
frequencies (SFs) rotating at various angular speeds. Compared with the reference stimulus with  
4
c/rev (0.64 c/rad), participants consistently overestimated the angular speed for test stimuli of  
higher radial SFs but underestimated that for a test stimulus of lower radial SFs. The spinner effect  
increased in magnitude but saturated rapidly as the test radial SF increased. Similar effects were  
observed with translating linear sinusoidal gratings of different SFs. Our results support the idea  
that human speed perception is biased by temporal frequency, which physically goes up as SF  
increases when the speed is held constant. Hence, the more dots or lines, the greater the  
perceived speed when they are moving coherently in a defined area.  
Keywords  
psychophysics, speed perception, visual illusion, visual motion  
Introduction  
Visual motion provides us with vital information about the environment necessary for our  
daily survival. The ability to perceive the speed and direction of external moving objects  
enables us to act on the objects or to navigate through the environment safely without being  
harmed. While detection of motion and identification of direction have been studied  
Corresponding author:  
Hiroshi Ashida, Graduate School of Letters, Kyoto University Sakyo, Kyoto 6068501, Japan.  
Email: ashida@psy.bun.kyoto-u.ac.jp  
Creative Commons CC-BY: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 License  
(http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0/) which permits any use, reproduction and distribution of the work without  
further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sage-  
pub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage).  
2
i-Perception  
extensively by psychophysicists, physiologists, and computational neuroscientists, our  
knowledge of speed perception is rather limited in terms of its phenomenology and  
underlying mechanisms (e.g., see a review by Nishida, 2011). One possible factor limiting  
our understanding of speed perception and vision in general is the inverse projection problem  
(
Palmer, 1999). This problem arises because an infinite number of distal environmental  
objects with different shapes and sizes seen in the three-dimensional (3D) world can cast  
the same two-dimensional (2D) optical image onto the retina, introducing ambiguities  
difficult to solve. A moving distal environmental object further extends the inverse  
projection problem into the time domain and possibly introduces more uncertainty and  
inaccuracy for the visual system in processing visual motion information. Since the claims  
that the human visual system analyses complex visual images by their spatial frequency (SF)  
content like a Fourier analyzer (Campbell & Robson, 1968; Sachs, Nachmias, & Robson,  
1
971), researchers have been using sinusoidal gratings extensively to study human vision. The  
speed of a moving stimulus used in researches is consequently expressed in terms of its spatial  
and temporal frequencies, and is susceptible to biases from other stimulus properties such as  
its luminance contrast (Thompson, Brooks, & Hammett, 2006; Thompson, 1982) and colour  
(
Cavanagh, Tyler, & Favreau, 1984).  
A counterintuitive bias in speed perception was recently demonstrated by Ho and Anstis  
2013) in the 2013 Best Illusion of the Year contest. They later redesigned it as ‘‘the spinner  
(
illusion’’ using simpler disk elements, as shown in Figure 1 (see also Appendix Movie 1). The  
four yellow dots on the left and the eight yellow dots on the right were both set to revolve at  
the same rate, yet all observers consistently reported seeing the dots on the right as rotating  
faster than those on the left. In the demonstration video, the number of dots on the right  
increases from 4 to 8 and then 12, with the rotation seeming faster with each increase.  
The spinner illusion is interesting because there is no obvious reason in physics why  
increasing the number of dots should increase their apparent speed, assuming that all the  
dots are clearly visible. Ho and Anstis (2013) originally suggested that the greater retinal blur  
caused by more dots might make them look faster. While it is known that motion streaks can  
enhance motion perception (Apthorp et al., 2013; Geisler, 1999), their influence on speed  
Figure 1. The spinner illusion. The four and eight yellow dots revolve around the blue disks at the same  
physical speed (top), but the perceived speed appears to be faster with eight dots than with four dots  
(bottom). Arrow lengths symbolize speeds. The blue disks were not included in the original illusion work, but  
are shown here for illustration purposes.  
Ashida et al.  
3
perception has not been established. We therefore measured the spinner effect with sine wave  
grating stimuli that are smooth and less susceptible to smearing.  
An alternative account would treat this illusion as a partial failure to compute speed from  
the temporal frequency (TF) and SF of the stimulus. While local speed is obtained by local  
TF divided by SF in theory, this computation might be performed only in an approximate  
way. It has been reported that perceived speed of translating one-dimensional (1D) sinusoidal  
gratings depends on their SF. Campbell and Maffei (1981) studied rotating 1D gratings and  
found that their perceived speed followed an inverted-U function of spatial frequency.  
Diener, Wist, Dichgans, and Brandt (1976) reported increased perceived speed at higher  
SFs measured by magnitude estimation, a finding that was later echoed by Chen, Bedell,  
and Frishman (1998) using a 2AFC procedure. Smith and Edger (1990) showed opposite  
results that both perceived speed and TF decreased as the SF increased, but they pointed out  
that the results may depend on the range of SFs used.  
In the spinner illusion (Figure 1), SF along the circular path of motion increases as the  
number, and thus density, of dots increases. Possibly this illusion directly reflects the  
spatiotemporal characteristics of the visual system as described earlier. If so, one extreme  
possibility would be that speed judgments of rotating stimuli are made on the basis of local  
TF, ignoring the SF. Or they might be made as a compromise between the speed and TF of a  
rotating stimulus. The results in the literature are not conclusive. One problem is that it is not  
straightforward to apply results from the linear gratings to the configuration of a rotating  
spinner illusion. We first need to confirm that simple sinusoidal patterns give similar illusions,  
and then to examine the spatiotemporal characteristics of the illusion parametrically.  
We therefore investigated the effect of SF on the spinner illusion in radial gratings  
Figure 2). First, we confirmed that the spinner illusion occurs with simple radial  
(
sinusoidal gratings for naıve participants (Experiment 1). Sinusoidal modulation is also  
¨
effective in minimizing the effect of blur, as image blur does not yield motion streaks but  
only reduces the effective contrast. Second, we examined whether the illusion depends more  
on speed itself or TF, by taking more detailed measurements from trained participants  
(
Experiment 2). Finally, we confirmed that a similar illusion occurs for translational  
motion of one-dimensional gratings (Experiment 3). We will then discuss the cause of this  
illusion in terms of general spatiotemporal integration.  
Experiment 1  
The point of subjective equality (PSE) in perceived speed for two simultaneously presented  
rotating radial sinusoidal gratings of different radial spatial frequencies (RSFs) was assessed  
psychophysically using a method of constant stimuli. Specifically, the rotating speed of the  
1
matching stimulus (RSF of 4 c/rev: cycle per revolution ) was varied randomly between trials  
and was compared with that of the test stimulus (RSF of either 8, 10, or 12 c/rev; used  
uniquely in separate experimental blocks) that was kept constant at 0.33 rev/s, in order to  
obtain psychometric functions for the estimation of their PSEs. We expected the resulting  
PSE to be higher than the veridical speed of the test stimuli (0.33 rev/s) if the spinner effect  
occurred.  
Methods  
Participants. Eight psychology undergraduate students (6 females and 2 males, aged 19–22)  
¨
in Kyoto University participated to fulfill partial requirement of a course. They were naıve  
4
i-Perception  
Figure 2. The basic stimulus configuration. Left: matching stimulus of 4 c/rev, Right: test stimulus of 12 c/rev  
as an example. Both stimuli rotated in the same direction. The test and matching sides as well as their yoked  
direction of rotation were randomized and balanced across trials. In Experiment 1: a ¼ 2.2 , b ¼ 8.8 , and  
c ¼ 5.5 . In Experiments 2 and 3: a ¼ 2.75 , b ¼ 11.0 , and c ¼ 6.9 .  
as to the specific purpose of the experiment, although they had seen a demonstration  
of the spinner illusion prior to the experiment. All had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.  
Apparatus and stimuli. Stimuli were generated and presented by using PsychToolBox 3  
Brainard, 1997; Pelli, 1997) on MATLAB (The Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA, USA).  
(
They were presented on one of the three monitors: two 24-inch (BenQ XL2411) and  
a 23-inch (Mitsubishi RDT233WX) LCD with 1920  1080 resolution and 60 Hz refresh  
rate, driven by PCs running Microsoft Windows 7. Participants rested their heads  
comfortably on a chin rest during experiment with a viewing distance at 50 cm. The  
2
2
mean luminance was 40 cd/m for one BenQ and 100 cd/m for the others. The  
luminance profile of each screen was measured and was linearized using a photometer  
(
Minolta LM1).  
The basic stimulus configuration is shown in Figure 2 (see also Appendix Movie 2). Two  
radial gratings, each presented inside a ring-shaped window subtending a visual angle of 8.8 ,  
were centered 5.5 laterally on each side of the central red fixation mark. The width of the  
rings was 2.2 . The two yoked stimuli always appeared and disappeared on the screen  
simultaneously. They had a sluggish temporal envelope: Their luminance contrast was  
increased linearly from 0% to 50% in 0.25 s and stayed at 50% for 0.5 s, and then  
decreased linearly back to 0% in 0.25 s. The two stimuli rotated in the same direction in  
each trial, and both directions were tested within each block of trials.  
The RSF for the matching stimulus was 4 c/rev (0.64 c/rad), while the RSF of the test  
stimulus was picked from three preset values of 8, 12, or 16 c/rev (1.27, 1.91, and 2.55 c/rad)  
and stayed constant within each block of trials.  
Ashida et al.  
5
(
a)  
(b)  
1.0  
1
00  
8
6
4
2
0
0
0
0
0
.5  
8
c/rev  
main  
1
1
2 c/rev  
6 c/rev  
mirrored  
0
0.1  
0.1  
0.2  
0.4  
0.6 0.8  
5
10  
15  
Matching Speed (rev/s)  
Radial Spatial Frequency (c/rev)  
Figure 3. The results of experiment 1: (a) pooled psychometric functions from all the participants. The  
speed of the 4 c/rev matching stimuli needed to be increased from 0.33 rev/s to match the speed of 8 to  
1
6 c/rev test stimuli (arrow). (b) The averaged PSE values (matched speeds) across participants, plotted as a  
function of RSF with 95% confidence intervals. The dashed straight line in each panel shows the actual speed  
of the test stimuli. (b) The filled symbols represent the main results, while the open symbols represent the  
auxiliary results with the speed of the higher SF stimuli manipulated; they are above and below the actual  
speed line in about the same distances in the log scale, so the two results agree quantitatively as to the  
amount of speed overestimation. The shifts of the psychometric functions (a) and thus the shifts of the  
matching speed (b) are all consistent with speed overestimation for higher RSFs.  
The speed for the test stimulus was fixed at 0.33 rev/s (2.09 rad/s) and the speed for the  
matching stimulus was picked randomly from seven preset values between 0.17 and 0.67 rev/s  
(
1.05–4.19 rad/s).  
Procedure  
The method of constant stimuli was used to present a yoked test-matching stimulus pair on  
each trial. The participants judged whether the radial grating inside the left or the right  
annulus window appeared faster in a 2AFC procedure and responded by pressing one of  
two designated computer keys.  
Trials were organized in blocks by the three test RSFs. One block consisted of 28 trials  
(
7 manipulated speeds  2 sides  2 rotation directions), and each block was repeated four  
times in a pseudorandom order, resulting in 16 measurements per manipulated matching  
speed. The computer programme randomized the manipulated speed variations, direction of  
stimulus-pair rotation, and the presentation sides of test-matching stimuli.  
Results and Discussion  
Probit analysis (Finney, 1971; with the scripts by Johnson, Dahlgren, Siegfried, & Ellis, 2013)  
was used to estimate each participant’s PSE in perceived speed for each test RSF. The results  
were collapsed across presentation sides of stimuli and their rotation directions. Figure 3(a)  
shows the averaged psychometric functions from all participants, and Figure 3(b) shows  
estimated PSE values for all test-matching paired stimuli. It is evident from the data that  
6
i-Perception  
the speed of the matching stimulus needed to be turned up beyond the test speed of 0.33 rev/s  
to match the perceived speeds of the test stimuli. In other words, the speed of a higher RSF  
test stimulus was consistently overestimated by the participants, which agreed with the  
original spinner illusion observation.  
Specifically, the participants required the 4 c/rev matching stimulus to rotate 32% to 36%  
faster than the actual speed of the high-RSF test stimuli in order to match the perceived  
speeds. There was no significant difference in the matched speeds among the test stimuli of 8,  
1
2, and 16 c/rev, F(2,14) ¼ 0.334, p ¼ 0.721 by repeated-measure ANOVA.  
The speed overestimation of the test stimuli (>30%) here is similar to the report of 33%  
for 8 versus 16 dot condition of the original spinner illusion (Anstis & Ho, 2014). It is,  
however, not consistent that our data from Experiment 1 did not reveal differences among  
the three high-RSF tests. A possible factor might be compression of perceived speed in a  
higher RSF range; tiny differences for higher RSFs may be buried in noise under the ceiling  
effect.  
We therefore ran an auxiliary experiment with a different speed manipulation in a mirrored  
fashion where the speeds of the higher RSF stimuli (8, 12, 16 c/rev) were manipulated  
to match the apparent speed of the 4 c/rev reference stimulus. The reference speed used  
here was the same as the test speed in the main experiment (0.33 rev/s ¼ 2.09 rad/s).  
We used the same set of apparatus, but used PsychoPy (Peirce, 2007) for  
stimulus generation and experimental control. The manipulated speed for the test  
stimulus was picked randomly from seven preset values between 0.17 and 0.47 rev/s  
(
¨
1.05–2.96 rad/s). Nine different naıve participants (2 females and 7 male psychology  
students, aged 19–22) participated to fulfill partial requirement of a course. Their results  
are shown in Figure 3(b) by open symbols. Similar to the findings from the main  
experiment, no significant difference in the estimated PSE values was observed among the 8,  
1
2, and 16 c/rev test stimuli, F(2,16) ¼ 1.063, p ¼ .369 by repeated-measure ANOVA.  
Participants required the speed of higher RSF test stimuli to be decreased to 72% to 76%  
of the actual 4 c/rev reference stimulus speed to perceptually match the perceived rotational  
speed of the yoked stimulus pair. By taking the reciprocals of these PSE values, the  
result corresponded to a speed overestimation of higher RSF stimuli by 31% to 39%, an  
observed spinner effect which is quantitatively consistent with the main results of 33% to  
3
6% obtained in the main experiment. Thus, the results here mirrored those of the main  
experiment, indicating that the flat results among the test RSFs is not an artefact of the  
ceiling effect.  
We then reasoned that the insignificant differences observed could possibly be due to a  
¨
high level of internal noise from our naıve participants, lack of regression precision due to  
rough sampling of test speeds, or possible dependence of the effects upon the test speed. In  
Experiment 2, we therefore further assessed the spinner effect more extensively using trained  
participants with a staircase method while adopting a wider range of test RSFs in  
combination with multiple test speeds as manipulated variables.  
Experiment 2  
Participants  
One of the authors and two psychophysically trained participants were tested (one female and  
¨
two males, aged 22–47). The two participants other than the author were naıve as to the  
specific purpose of this experiment, and were paid for their time at the university standard  
rate. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.  
Ashida et al.  
7
Apparatus and Stimuli  
The stimuli were generated by using PsychoPy 1.82 (Peirce, 2007), running on an Apple  
MacBook Pro 15, and were presented on a 19-inch CRT (EIZO T761) with 1024  768  
resolution and 75 Hz refresh rate. The screen was viewed from the distance of 45 cm with  
the aid of a chin rest. Luminance profile of the screen was measured and linearized with a  
photometer (Photo Research PR-655).  
The basic stimulus configuration was the same as in Experiment 1 but 25% larger in size;  
the ring-shaped windows subtended 11 , with the annulus subtending 2.75 in width  
(
Figure 2). These two stimulus windows were centered laterally at an eccentricity of 6.9  
on both sides of the central fixation mark. The time course of stimulus presentation was  
the same, too: linear increase for 0.25 s, staying at 50% for 0.5 s, and linear decrease to zero  
for 0.25 s.  
The RSF of the matching stimulus used here was again 4 c/rev (0.64 c/rad) as used in  
Experiment 1. Five test SFs were used: 2, 8, 12, 16, and 20 c/rev (0.32, 1.27, 1.91, 2.55,  
3
3
.18 c/rad) in combination with three test speeds: 0.17, 0.33, and 0.50 rev/s (1.05, 2.09, and  
.14 rad/s).  
Procedure  
Participants were given the same 2AFC discrimination task as described in Experiment 1,  
judging the faster stimulus. All participants were tested with 15 different test stimulus  
conditions (i.e., the five test RSFs  three test speeds as stated earlier) to complete this  
experiment. A staircase method (1-up and 1-down) with smaller preset step sizes was used  
for capturing more precise changes in participants’ responses around the PSE for each test  
stimulus.  
Each condition was tested in separate blocks of randomly interleaved double staircases  
one staircase for one direction of motion). Each staircase was terminated after 28 trials  
i.e., 56 trials per run with double staircases). Each block was repeated three times,  
(
(
bringing a grand total of 168 trials per stimulus condition.  
Results  
Responses were pooled for each test stimulus condition within individual participant. The  
perceived matched speed (PSE) for each test stimulus condition was estimated with 95%  
confidence intervals using probit analysis (Finney, 1971; Johnson et al., 2013).  
Figure 4 shows the results for all three participants. Matched speeds (PSEs) of the  
matching stimulus to the test stimuli are plotted as a function of RSF for each participant  
in separate panels. Since the 4 c/rev stimulus was used as the matching stimulus, we assumed  
the veridical speed to be its perceived speed for each test speed level (marked by larger  
symbols in Figure 4). The results look very similar across all three participants. Most of  
the test stimuli of 8 c/rev RSF or greater required the matching speed to be set above the  
corresponding actual test speed, demonstrating overestimated perceived speed of higher RSF  
test stimuli as found in Experiment 1. On the other hand, the matching speeds for the 2 c/rev  
test stimulus all fell below their veridical speed lines, revealing an underestimation of  
perceived speed for the lower RSF stimulus.  
All the PSE curves fell within an area between the constant-TF and veridical test speed  
lines, and tended toward horizontal as the RSF of the test stimuli increased. These data reveal  
8
i-Perception  
1
1
0
0
.5  
.0  
.5  
.0  
S1  
S2  
S3  
const. const.  
speed TF  
results  
0
0
0
.50 rev/s  
.33 rev/s  
.17 rev/s  
0
5
10  
15  
20  
0
5
10  
15  
20  
0
5
10  
15  
20  
Radial Spatial Frequency (c/rev)  
Figure 4. Results of Experiment 2 for all three participants in linear scales. PSE speeds are plotted as  
a function of the test RSF for each test speed. Error bars show 95% confidence intervals. The larger data  
symbols at 4 c/rev denote the matching stimulus itself where its own PSEs were not measured. Pale solid  
horizontal lines depict the physical speeds of the three test stimuli, and the pale dashed lines depict the  
physical speeds that give constant TFs (i.e., hypothetical lines of matched speeds if the perceived speeds  
of test stimuli were determined solely by the local TF characteristics.).  
that the participants made a compromise between the actual speed and the TF of the moving  
stimuli in making their speed judgments on the revolving stimuli. The response functions are  
also very similar in trend across the three tested speed levels for all tested RSFs, with PSE  
values increasing as the RSF and the test speed increase. To compare the results across test  
speeds, each participant’s PSE data are normalized with the test speeds (i.e., divided by  
corresponding test speed) and replotted in a log-log scale as presented in Figure 5(a). Note  
that this normalized plot shows quadratic trends for all three test speeds; log-quadratic  
2
functions were fitted well to the averaged data across participants (r > .99). The three  
curves nearly superimposed on each other except for the one obtained at the lowest test  
speed (0.17 rev/s), where PSEs for the lowest and highest RSFs were much lower than  
those for the faster test speeds. It is also noticed from the other two fitted curves that  
participants’ overestimation on perceived speeds for the higher RSF test stimuli started to  
level off at 8 c/rev. There are only subtle differences in speed overestimation among the  
stimuli of 8 to 16 c/rev, confirming our earlier finding in Experiment 1. We also noticed  
that the results from the three participants were quantitatively consistent except for data  
points at either end of RSFs examined.  
Furthermore, from Figure 5(a), we see that perceived speed on the 2 c/rev stimulus lies  
much closer to the oblique TF line than the horizontal line, a characteristic that is opposite  
for the higher RSF stimuli. This means that speed perception is more heavily weighted by TF  
at low RSFs but more by the actual stimulus speed at high RSFs. To quantify such changes in  
weighing on speed perception by the TF characteristics of a revolving stimulus, we evaluated  
the contribution of TF by the following equation that can account for the matched speed as a  
weighted sum of the constant-TF speed and the stimulus’ actual angular speed:  
m ¼ b  STF þ ð1  bÞS  
Ashida et al.  
9
(
a)  
(b)  
1
.8  
.6  
.4  
.2  
0
0
0
0
2
.0  
.0  
0.17 rev/s  
y=-0.42 log(x)+1.24  
1
0.33 rev/s  
y=-0.31 log(x)+0.99  
0.50 rev/s  
const. TF  
y=-0.24 log(x)+0.79  
const. speed  
0.50 rps  
S1  
S2  
S3  
0.33 rps  
0.17 rps  
0
.2  
0
1
10  
1
10  
Radial Spatial Frequency (c/rev)  
Radial Spatial Frequency (c/rev)  
Figure 5. (a) The results of Experiment 2 normalized with the corresponding test speed in a log-log scale,  
with different symbols representing results of each participant (adapted from Figure 4). The large black square  
denotes the matching speed, which corresponds to the larger symbols in Figure 4. Solid curves show fitted  
2
double-log quadratic functions to the averaged data across participants for each test speed (r > 0.99 for all).  
The 4 c/rev stimulus was used as the matching stimulus, and therefore no PSE estimate was made. The gray  
lines show equal speeds (solid) and equal-TFs (dashed). (b) Estimated TF bias for each test speed. Colored  
2
lines show fitted linear functions (r > 0.98 for all) for TF weighting (b). Dotted lines represent the weights on  
actual speed (1  b).  
where m is the matched speed for a revolving stimulus, b is the TF-bias weighing factor, STF is  
the constant-TF speed at each spatial frequency, and S is the veridical stimulus speed. TF  
would have no effect with b ¼ 0 and the strongest effect with b ¼ 1. Figure 5(b) shows b (solid  
lines) and 1  b (dotted lines) as a function of the RSF, which were obtained by solving this  
equation for the averaged results in Figure 5(a) as m for each test speed. This plot reveals that  
the TF weighting on speed perception falls almost linearly in a log scale as the RSF of a  
stimulus increases. Even though the speed overestimation effect increases for stimuli of higher  
RSFs, the effect of TF on speed perception is actually stronger for stimuli of lower RSFs.  
This plot also indicates that the effect of TF is larger for a slower speed at the lowest RSF  
tested; the reason is not clear, but it could be an artefact of showing very thick stripes in  
narrow rings.  
The curves may be subject to change by the choice of the matching stimulus. An auxiliary  
experiment performed by S1, however, suggested that such a nonlinear effect, if any, might  
not be substantial at least for our stimulus set; the speed of the 8 c/rev stimulus was matched  
to that of the 16 c/rev stimulus by the same procedure, and the speed of 16 c/rev stimulus was  
overestimated by 9.2% with the 95% confidence interval from 5.9% to 12.5%. The data from  
S1 in Figure 4 increased by 11.5% from 8 c/rev to 16 c/rev, which falls within this confidence  
interval.  
In short, the spinner effect is observed with radial sinusoidal stimuli. Perceived speed  
of a test stimulus does increase with the RSF of the stimulus, weighted concomitantly  
by an increasing TF. But the magnitude of the spinner effect reaches a plateau relatively  
quickly at higher RSFs, indicating that the bias toward TF is actually smaller for  
higher RSFs. Therefore, possible difference among 8 to 16 c/rev stimuli could be  
1
0
i-Perception  
easily buried in noise in Experiment 1 with naı  
¨
speeds.  
ve participants and with coarser grain of test  
Experiment 3  
If the spinner illusion reflects a genuine SF dependent speed overestimation, it may not be  
specific to rotational motion. We can actually experience an analogous speed illusion in the  
demonstration movie with translating linear gratings (Appendix Movie 3), as also predicted  
from previous reports (Chen et al., 1998; Diener et al., 1976). In Experiment 3, we therefore  
assessed if analogous characteristics of the spinner illusion would be found in drifting vertical  
sine wave gratings. The SFs of the 1D gratings used here were roughly matched to the SF of  
the radial stimuli in Experiment 2.  
Methods  
Participants. The same three participants as in Experiment 2 were tested.  
Apparatus and stimuli. The apparatus was the same as in Experiment 2, also controlled with  
PsychoPy 1.82 (Peirce, 2007). The layout of the paired matching-test stimulus was similar to  
that used in Experiment 2. Here, two 1D vertical sinusoidal grating of different SFs, always  
drifting in the same direction, were presented inside two separate, 11 diameter circular  
windows on both sides of the central fixation point. Their maximum luminance contrast  
was 50%, presented to participants using the same trapezoidal time envelope as before.  
The SFs were approximately matched to those of the radial gratings as follows;  
luminances were modulated horizontally in the frequencies of 2, 4, 8, 12, and 16 cycles per  
the circumference of the middle point of the ring in Experiment 2 (i.e., along the circle of  
8
.25 diameter), resulting in 0.08, 0.15, 0.31, 0.46, and 0.62 c/deg, respectively. The SF of the  
matching stimulus was 0.15 c/deg, and that of the test stimulus was one of the other four SFs.  
A single standard speed of 8.64 deg/s (corresponding to 0.33 rev/s ¼ 2.09 rad/s of the radial  
grating at the middle radius of the ring) was tested.  
Procedure. The task and the design was the same as in Experiment 2; each test SF was tested  
separately in a double-random-staircase run of 28 trials for each direction, and each run was  
repeated three times in a random order.  
Results  
Probit analysis (Finney, 1971; Johnson et al., 2013) was used to estimate the matched speed at  
9
5% confidence intervals. Figure 6(a) shows the matched speed as a function of the test SF.  
Again, the PSE values fell between the area bounded by speed with constant TF and the  
veridical base speed of the test stimuli as shown in Figure 5(a), indicating overestimations and  
underestimations of perceived speed in higher and lower test SFs, respectively, when  
compared with the 0.15 c/deg matching stimulus.  
Corresponding data from Experiment 2 were converted in the way as described in the  
Apparatus and Stimuli section and were plotted together in Figure 6(a). The pattern of results  
2
is very similar to that of the radial stimuli, as the fitted log-quadratic function (r > .99) is  
comparable to the adapted curve from Figure 5(a). While the overestimation was somewhat  
larger for the linear gratings, this difference is surprisingly small, given many possible  
confounds such as imperfect matching of SFs, different stimulus windows, or larger SF  
Ashida et al.  
11  
(
a)  
(b)  
1
.8  
.6  
.4  
.2  
0
2
1
0
0
0
0
0
0
linear  
y=-0.25 log(x)+0.086  
radial  
y=-0.33 log(x)-0.468  
S1  
S2  
S3  
const. TF  
const. speed  
average linear  
average radial  
2
0
.05  
0.1  
0.2  
0.4  
0.05  
0.1  
0.2  
0.4  
Spatial Frequency (c/deg)  
Spatial Frequency (c/deg)  
Figure 6. Results of Experiment 3. (a) Matched speed of the 0.15 c/deg stimulus is plotted as a function of  
the test spatial frequency. Symbols show individual results with 95% confidence intervals. The larger square  
shows the point of matching SF (not measured). The thick black curve denotes the fitted log-quadratic  
function to the averaged data. The fitted curve for the 4 c/rev condition in Figure 5(a) was adapted and  
superimposed as a dotted curve. (b) Estimated TF weighting, plotted as a function of spatial frequency.  
2
A linear function was fitted to the data (r > .99). Corresponding data for the radial stimuli in Experiment 2  
(
standard speed of 0.33 rev/s) were adapted from Figure 5(b) and superimposed (gray squares and gray lines).  
The dashed lines represent bias for speed (1  b).  
artefacts at the stimulus edges of linear gratings, which are inevitable for the different kinds of  
stimuli. This result suggests that the spinner illusion is a consequence of general speed  
computation from spatiotemporal frequencies, rather than manifestation of some  
idiosyncratic rotation-specific effects.  
Figure 6(b) depicts the TF weighting on perceived speeds of drifting gratings in the same  
way as in Figure 5(b) but as a function of linear SF. The data for the corresponding condition  
in Experiment 2 with radial gratings (standard speed of 0.33 rev/s) are adapted from  
Figure 5(b) and are plotted along in this figure. The decline of TF weighting is very  
similar for the two types of stimuli. The small difference in the slopes may be due to  
several factors as noted earlier.  
We should also note that the shape of the curve is similar to that of speed matching by  
Jogan and Stocker (2015; their Figure 5(b), test stimuli of A–D), although they did not  
discuss this ‘‘single-channel’’ response in detail, because their focus was on integration of  
such responses in compound SF stimuli.  
General Discussion  
The spinner illusion demonstrates that perceived speed is affected by the number of dots even  
when they move at a constant speed. We have confirmed that this effect generalizes to radial  
and linear forms of sinusoidal gratings. The spinner illusion therefore is considered to reflect  
a general speed overestimation for high SFs that has been reported in the literature. The effect  
of motion blur, if any, is not a necessary condition.  
1
2
i-Perception  
The spinner illusion as a TF bias in spatiotemporal integration  
As the spinner illusion occurs in a very similar way for radial and linear gratings, the effect is  
consistent with a number of studies that showed higher perceived speed for higher SFs with  
linear gratings (Brooks, Morris, & Thompson, 2011; Campbell & Maffei, 1981; Chen et al.,  
1
998; Diener et al., 1976; McKee & Silverman, 1986). The asymptotic curve in our Figure 6(a)  
parallels with Figure 6 in McKee & Silverman (1986) and Figure 3 of Brooks et al. (2011).  
Smith and Edgar showed opposite results of speed underestimation for high SFs, but as  
they discussed (1990, p. 1473), their results are not necessarily in conflict because they used  
higher range of SFs (0.5–2 c/deg) than ours (0.08–0.68 c/deg) and Diener et al.’s (0.01–  
0.07 c/deg); the overall SF tuning might be inverted U-shaped as observed in Campbell  
and Maffei (1981).  
Taken together, these results support a bias toward TF in the spinner illusion, as Anstis  
and Ho (2014) proposed. TF should increase along with increasing SF in order to keep the  
speed constant, and this higher TF could bias speed coding. Although we can make speed and  
¨
TF judgments independently (Smith & Edgar, 1990, p. 1469), naıve judgments may be  
erroneously contaminated by the TF. This, however, does not seem to explain our  
compelling perception of speed difference in the spinner illusion demonstrations. Also, the  
¨
results were consistent across naıve and trained participants in our experiments. The TF bias  
therefore could be an innate property in speed perception.  
It is conceived that the visual system initially takes separate measures in space and time  
(
i.e., SF and TF), and then integrates these measures into the metric of speed at the level  
of V1 complex cells to the area MT/V5 both in macaques (Priebe, Cassanello, &  
Lisberger, 2003; Priebe, Lisberger, & Movshon, 2006) and also in humans (Lingnau,  
Ashida, Wall, & Smith, 2009). Integration across space and time is then required for  
perceiving global pattern of linear or circular motion (e.g., Morrone, Burr, & Vaina,  
1
995). While the TF bias may arise at any of these stages, the results of similar spinner  
effects on circular and translating motions might suggest that it occurs before the stage of  
global integration.  
The exact mechanism of the TF bias in speed perception is yet to be investigated, but our  
results of stronger TF bias for lower SFs might be explained by hypothetical receptive fields  
that are not large enough for the low-SF stimuli. The nominal SF of our stimuli extends  
down to 0.08 c/deg, that is, 12.5 deg/c. This is larger than the human population receptive  
field sizes, measured by using fMRI, in most visual areas including putative human MT and  
MST up to the eccentricity of 10 in the periphery (Amano, Wandell, & Dumoulin, 2009).  
Appendix Movie 4 shows a hypothetical receptive field that covers less than one cycle of a  
low-SF drifting grating, which is therefore seen as mostly flickering with very little motion  
signal. On the other hand, the same receptive field can register several moving bars of a high-  
SF grating, so the direction of motion is now unambiguous—and also looks fast. This simple  
model explains both the basic spinner effect, that fine bars appear to move faster than coarse  
bars, and also the fact that speed judgments of coarse moving bars are more strongly  
weighted by TF than by actual velocity.  
Note that the illusion could be understood in accordance with the gradient-based models  
of motion detection (e.g., Anstis, 1967, 1990; Johnston, McOwan, & Buxton, 1992; Marr &  
Ullman, 1981). In the case of sinusoidal modulation, because d/dt[sin(ft)] ¼ fcos(ft),  
maximum temporal gradient is proportional to TF. Speed perception could be understood  
as biased toward temporal gradient instead of TF.  
Ashida et al.  
13  
Ecological Interpretations  
For periodic grating patterns, SF (or RSF) is a reciprocal of the size of each bar. The spinner  
effect therefore parallels the classic report of Brown (1931) that ‘‘other things being equal  
larger figures are phenomenally slower’’ (p. 222). It is, however, not fully understood why  
speed perception depends on size. A possible account could refer to speed constancy across  
distances; as an object comes closer, the retinal size and speed increase when the physical  
speed is constant. While Brown (1931) argued that constancy of velocity (i.e., speed) is not  
fully deducible from size constancy, Rock, Hill, and Fineman (1968) concluded that size  
constancy and speed constancy are indeed related, from the results of experiments without  
visible frames of references.  
The rotating spinner illusion is not readily explained by speed constancy because  
RSF does not change across distances. On the other hand, the two-dimensional SF  
components along the x-y coordinates do change with distance for both linear or radial  
gratings, while TF remains constant. TF is therefore a more invariant measure than SF  
across distances, which may be one reason for more dependence on TF in speed  
computations.  
Other possible explanations might refer to the statistical tendency that lower SFs are likely  
more stable than higher SFs in the scene. This is related to the hypothesis of Bayesian prior  
for slow motion (Jogan & Stocker, 2015; Vintch & Gardner, 2014; Weiss, Simoncelli, &  
Adelson, 2002), reflecting the fact that the world is mostly stable while smaller objects can  
move around, although this might not always hold (Hammett, Champion, Thompson,  
Morland, 2007).  
Limitations  
There remains a question: Why does the speed appear to get faster steadily whenever the  
number of discs increase in the original spinner illusion, while the perceived speed saturated  
rapidly in our experiments? A potential cause for the discrepancy might be the high-SF  
harmonic components in the dot stimuli, as Brooks et al. (2011) showed that a complex  
grating of 1f þ 2f SF components yielded less asymptotic curves of speed overestimation  
for higher SFs than a simple grating. Smith and Edgar (1991) also revealed nonlinear ways  
of combining element speeds in various complex gratings, but how this is related to the  
spinner illusion remains open for further investigation. Another factor could be the retinal  
blur due to sharp edges that might add to the effect, as Ho and Anstis (2013) originally  
postulated.  
The physical contrast was always held constant at 50% in our experiments, and there  
could have been a confound of perceived contrast across SFs, since perceived speed depends  
on contrast (Thompson, 1982). We do not, however, consider that this effect is crucial in our  
case, because the effect of contrast is less clear for high-contrast stimuli. While Stone  
and Thompson (1992) showed that the effect does not saturate up to 70% contrast, Smith  
and Edgar (1990) informally noted that the perceived speed was independent of contrast  
above 10%.  
Another remaining question is the generality of the spinner effect for second-order stimuli  
that cannot be computed from spatial and temporal frequencies, which is currently under  
investigation by one of the authors (A. H.).  
1
4
i-Perception  
Author Note  
Preliminary results were presented at ECVP 2014 by H. A. and A. K.  
Declaration of Conflicting Interests  
The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or  
publication of this article.  
Funding  
The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or  
publication of this article: This study was supported by JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Scientific Research  
(
KAKENHI) #15H01984 for AK and HA, and #26285165 for HA. SA was supported by a grant  
from the UCSD Department of Psychology. AH was supported by a professional development grant  
from the Ambrose University.  
Supplemental Material  
Note  
1
. We use the unit of c/rev for RSF because it is more intuitive and directly comparable to the number  
of dots in the original spinner illusion. We also use the unit of rev/s for speed, for consistency.  
Corresponding values in SI units (c/rad and rad/s, respectively) are presented in parentheses in the  
methods sections.  
References  
Amano, K., Wandell, B. A., & Dumoulin, S. O. (2009). Visual field maps, population receptive field  
sizes, and visual field coverage in the human MTþ complex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102,  
2704–2718. doi:10.1152/jn.00102.2009  
Anstis, S. (1990). Motion aftereffects from a motionless stimulus. Perception, 19, 301–306.  
Anstis, S. (1967). Visual adaptation to gradual change of intensity. Science, 155, 710–712.  
Anstis, S., & Ho, A. (2014). Apparent speed of a rotating disk varies with texture density. Journal of  
Vision, 14, 1333. doi:10.1167/14.10.1333  
Apthorp, D., Schwarzkopf, D. S., Kaul, C., Bahrami, B., Alais, D., & Rees, G. (2013). Direct evidence  
for encoding of motion streaks in human visual cortex. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London.  
B: Biological Sciences, 280, 20122339. doi:10.1098/rspb.2012.2339  
Brainard, D. H. (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vision, 10, 433–436.  
Brooks, K. R., Morris, T., & Thompson, P. (2011). Contrast and stimulus complexity moderate the  
relationship between spatial frequency and perceived speed: Implications for MT models of speed  
perception. Journal of Vision, 11, 19. doi:10.1167/11.14.19  
Brown, J. F. (1931). The visual perception of velocity. Psychologische Forschung, 14, 199–232.  
doi:10.1007/BF00403873  
Campbell, F. W., & Maffei, L. (1981). The influence of spatial frequency and contrast on the perception  
of moving patterns. Vision Research, 21, 713–721.  
Campbell, F. W., & Robson, J. G. (1968). Application of Fourier analysis to the visibility of gratings.  
The Journal of Physiology, 197, 551–566.  
Cavanagh, P., Tyler, C. W., & Favreau, O. E. (1984). Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings.  
Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 1, 893–899.  
Ashida et al.  
15  
Chen, Y., Bedell, H. E., & Frishman, L. J. (1998). The precision of velocity discrimination across spatial  
frequency. Percept Psychophys, 60, 1329–1336.  
Diener, H. C., Wist, E. R., Dichgans, J., & Brandt, T. (1976). The spatial frequency effect on perceived  
velocity. Vision Research, 16, 169–176.  
Finney, D. J. (1971). Probit analysis 3rd ed. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.  
Geisler, W. S. (1999). Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction. Nature, 400, 65–69.  
doi:10.1038/21886  
Hammett, S. T., Champion, R. A., Thompson, P. G., & Morland, A. B. (2007). Perceptual distortions  
of speed at low luminance: Evidence inconsistent with a Bayesian account of speed encoding. Vision  
Research, 47, 564–568. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2006.08.013  
Ho, A., & Anstis, S. (2013). The Coyote Illusion: Motion blur increases apparent speed. Paper presented  
Jogan, M., & Stocker, A. A. (2015). Signal integration in human visual speed perception. Journal of  
Neuroscience, 35, 9381–9390. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4801-14.2015  
Johnson, R. M., Dahlgren, L., Siegfried, B. D., & Ellis, M. D. (2013). Acaricide, fungicide and drug  
interactions in honey bees (Apis mellifera). PLoS One, 8, e54092. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0054092  
Johnston, A., McOwan, P. W., & Buxton, H. (1992). A computational model of the analysis of some  
first-order and second-order motion patterns by simple and complex cells. Proceedings of the Royal  
Society of London. B: Biological Sciences, 250, 297–306. doi:10.1098/rspb.1992.0162  
Lingnau, A., Ashida, H., Wall, M. B., & Smith, A. T. (2009). Speed encoding in human visual cortex  
revealed by fMRI adaptation. Journal of Vision, 9, 3.1–3.14. doi:10.1167/9.13.3  
Marr, D., & Ullman, S. (1981). Directional selectivity and its use in early visual processing. Proceedings  
of the Royal Society of London. B: Biological Sciences, 211, 151–180.  
McKee, S. P., Silverman, G. H., & Nakayama, K. (1986). Precise velocity discrimination despite  
random variations in temporal frequency and contrast. Vision Research, 26, 609–619.  
Morrone, M. C., Burr, D. C., & Vaina, L. M. (1995). Two stages of visual processing for radial and  
circular motion. Nature, 376, 507–509. doi:10.1038/376507a0  
Nishida, S. (2011). Advancement of motion psychophysics: Review 2001–2010. Journal of Vision, 11,  
11. doi:10.1167/11.5.11  
Palmer, S. (1999). Vision Science: From Photons to Phenomenology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.  
Peirce, J. W. (2007). PsychoPy—Psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods,  
162, 8–13. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017  
Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into  
movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.  
Priebe, N. J., Cassanello, C. R., & Lisberger, S. G. (2003). The neural representation of speed in  
macaque area MT/V5. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5650–5661.  
Priebe, N. J., Lisberger, S. G., & Movshon, J. A. (2006). Tuning for spatiotemporal frequency and  
speed in directionally selective neurons of macaque striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 26,  
2
941–2950. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3936-05.2006  
Rock, I., Hill, A. L., & Fineman, M. (1968). Speed constancy as a function of size constancy. Perception  
Psychophysics, 4, 37–40.  
&
Sachs, M. B., Nachmias, J., & Robson, J. G. (1971). Spatial-frequency channels in human vision.  
Journal of the Optical Society of America, 61, 1176–1186.  
Smith, A. T., & Edgar, G. K. (1990). The influence of spatial frequency on perceived temporal  
frequency and perceived speed. Vision Research, 30, 1467–1474.  
Smith, A. T., & Edgar, G. K. (1991). Perceived speed and direction of complex gratings and plaids.  
Journal of the Optical Society of America A, 8, 1161–1171.  
Stone, L. S., & Thompson, P. (1992). Human speed perception is contrast dependent. Vision Research,  
32, 1535–1549.  
Thompson, P., Brooks, K., & Hammett, S. T. (2006). Speed can go up as well as down at low contrast:  
implications for models of motion perception. Vision Research, 46, 782–786. doi:10.1016/  
j.visres.2005.08.005  
1
6
i-Perception  
Thompson, P. G. (1982). Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast. Vision Research, 22,  
77–380.  
3
Vintch, B., & Gardner, J. L. (2014). Cortical correlates of human motion perception biases. Journal of  
Neuroscience, 34, 2592–2604. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2809-13.2014  
Weiss, Y., Simoncelli, E. P., & Adelson, E. H. (2002). Motion illusions as optimal percepts. Nature  
Neuroscience, 5, 598–604. doi:10.1038/nn858  
Author Biographies  
Hiroshi Ashida received his PhD in psychology from the Kyoto  
University, Japan. He has been working at Kyoto University  
since 2001 and became a professor in 2015. His main research  
interest is in visual processing of motion and visual illusion in  
general, studied with psychophysics and MRI.  
Alan Ho was born in Hong Kong. He did his postdoctoral work  
with Stuart Anstis at UCSD after obtaining his PhD in  
psychology from the Florida State University. His primary  
research interest is in visual motion perception. The ‘‘Coyote  
Illusion’’ that Alan and Stuart reported in 2013 was selected to  
be a Top 10 Illusion of the Year. He is currently an associate  
professor of psychology at the Ambrose University, Calgary,  
Canada.  
Akiyoshi Kitaoka is a professor of psychology in Ritsumeikan  
University, Kyoto/Osaka, Japan. He studies visual illusions and  
produces a variety of illusion works and exhibits them in his  
webpages or SNSs.  
Stuart Anstis was born in England and was a scholar at  
Winchester and Cambridge. Since his PhD at Cambridge with  
Richard Gregory, he has taught at the Universities of Bristol  
(
UK), York (Toronto), and California, San Diego (UCSD). He  
has published about 170 peer-reviewed papers. He is a visiting  
fellow at Pembroke College, Oxford, and a Humboldt fellow  
and received the Kurt-Koffka Medal for outstanding  
contributions to Vision Science in 2013.